James P. Scanlan, Attorney at Law

Home Page

Curriculum Vitae

Publications

Published Articles

Conference Presentations

Working Papers

page1

Journal Comments

Truth in Justice Articles

Measurement Letters

Measuring Health Disp

Outline and Guide to MHD

Summary to MHD

Solutions

page3

Solutions Database

Irreducible Minimums

Pay for Performance

Between Group Variance

Concentration Index

Gini Coefficient

Reporting Heterogeneity

Cohort Considerations

Relative v Absolute Diff

Whitehall Studies

AHRQ's Vanderbilt Report

NHDR Measurement

NHDR Technical Issues

MHD A Articles

MHD B Conf Presentations

MHD D Journal Comments

Consensus/Non-Consensus

Spurious Contradictions

Institutional Corresp

page2

Scanlan's Rule

Outline and Guide to SR

Summary to SR

Bibliography

Semantic Issues

Employment Tests

Case Study

Case Study Answers

Case Study II

Subgroup Effects

Subgroup Effects NC

Illogical Premises

Illogical Premises II

Inevitable Interaction

Interactions by Age

Literacy Illustration

RERI

Feminization of Poverty S

Explanatory Theories

Mortality and Survival

Truncation Issues

Collected Illustrations

Income Illustrations

Framingham Illustrations

Life Table Illustrations

NHANES Illustrations

Mort/Surv Illustration

Credit Score Illustration

Intermediate Outcomes

Representational Disp

Statistical Signif SR

Comparing Averages

Meta-Analysis

Case Control Studies

Criminal Record Effects

Sears Case Illustration

Numeracy Illustration

Obesity Illusration

LIHTC Approval Disparitie

Recidivism Illustration

Consensus

Algorithm Fairness

Mortality and Survival 2

Mort/Survival Update

Measures of Association

Immunization Disparities

Race Health Initiative

Educational Disparities

Disparities by Subject

CUNY ISLG Eq Indicators

Harvard CRP NCLB Study

New York Proficiency Disp

Education Trust GC Study

Education Trust HA Study

AE Casey Profic Study

McKinsey Achiev Gap Study

California RICA

Nuclear Deterrence

Employment Discrimination

Job Segregation

Measuring Hiring Discr

Disparate Impact

Four-Fifths Rule

Less Discr Alt - Proc

Less Discr Altl - Subs

Fisher v. Transco Serv

Jones v. City of Boston

Bottom Line Issue

Lending Disparities

Inc & Cred Score Example

Disparities - High Income

Underadjustment Issues

Absolute Differences - L

Lathern v. NationsBank

US v. Countrywide

US v. Wells Fargo

Partial Picture Issues

Foreclosure Disparities

File Comparison Issues

FHA/VA Steering Study

CAP TARP Study

Disparities by Sector

Holder/Perez Letter

Federal Reserve Letter

Discipline Disparities

COPAA v. DeVos

Kerri K. V. California

Truancy Illustration

Disparate Treatment

Relative Absolute Diff

Offense Type Issues

Los Angeles SWPBS

Oakland Disparities

Richmond Disparities

Nashville Disparities

California Disparities

Denver Disparities

Colorado Disparities

Nor Carolina Disparitie

Aurora Disparities

Allegheny County Disp

Evansville Disparities

Maryland Disparities

St. Paul Disparities

Seattle Disparities

Minneapolis Disparities

Oregon Disparities

Beaverton Disparities

Montgomery County Disp

Henrico County Disparitie

Florida Disparities

Connecticut Disparities

Portland Disparities

Minnesota Disparities

Massachusetts Disparities

Rhode Island Disparities

South Bend Disparities

Utah Disparities

Loudoun Cty Disparities

Kern County Disparities

Milwaukee Disparities

Urbana Disparities

Illinois Disparities

Virginia Disparities

Behavior

Suburban Disparities

Preschool Disparities

Restraint Disparities

Disabilities - PL 108-446

Keep Kids in School Act

Gender Disparities

Ferguson Arrest Disp

NEPC Colorado Study

NEPC National Study

California Prison Pop

APA Zero Tolerance Study

Flawed Inferences - Disc

Oakland Agreement

DOE Equity Report

IDEA Data Center Guide

Duncan/Ali Letter

Crim Justice Disparities

U.S. Customs Search Disp

Deescalation Training

Career Criminal Study

Implicit Bias Training

Drawing Inferences

Diversion Programs

Minneapolis PD Investig

Offense Type Issues CJD

Innumerate Decree Monitor

Massachusetts CJ Disparit

Feminization of Poverty

Affirmative Action

Affirm Action for Women

Other Affirm Action

Justice John Paul Stevens

Statistical Reasoning

The Sears Case

Sears Case Documents

The AT&T Consent Decree

Cross v. ASPI

Vignettes

Times Higher Issues

Gender Diff in DADT Term

Adjustment Issues

Percentage Points

Odds Ratios

Statistical Signif Vig

Journalists & Statistics

Multiplication Definition

Prosecutorial Misconduct

Outline and Guide

Misconduct Summary

B1 Agent Cain Testimony

B1a Bev Wilsh Diversion

B2 Bk Entry re Cain Call

B3 John Mitchell Count

B3a Obscuring Msg Slips

B3b Missing Barksdale Int

B4 Park Towers

B5 Dean 1997 Motion

B6 Demery Testimony

B7 Sankin Receipts

B7a Sankin HBS App

B8 DOJ Complicity

B9 Doc Manager Complaints

B9a Fabricated Gov Exh 25

B11a DC Bar Complaint

Letters (Misconduct)

Links Page

Misconduct Profiles

Arlin M. Adams

Jo Ann Harris

Bruce C. Swartz

Swartz Addendum 2

Swartz Addendum 3

Swartz Addendum 4

Swartz Addendum 7

Robert E. O'Neill

O'Neill Addendum 7

Paula A. Sweeney

Robert J. Meyer

Lantos Hearings

Password Protected

OIC Doc Manager Material

DC Bar Materials

Temp Confidential

DV Issues

Indexes

Document Storage

Pre 1989

1989 - present

Presentations

Prosec Misc Docs

Prosec Misc Docs II

Profile PDFs

Misc Letters July 2008 on

Large Prosec Misc Docs

HUD Documents

Transcripts

Miscellaneous Documents

Unpublished Papers

Letters re MHD

Tables

MHD Comments

Figures

ASPI Documents

Web Page PDFs

Sears Documents

Pages Transfer


LIFE TABLE ILLUSTRATIONS

(May 19, 2011, rev. Jan. 6, 2012)

 

Prefatory note:  This item is related to the Irreducible Minimums and Cohort Considerations sub-pages of the Measuring Health Disparities page and the Truncation Issues sub-page of the Scanlan’s Rule page.  The item uses life table information to illustrate the way measures of differences between outcome rates are affected by the overall prevalence of an outcome, something for which information across the life course it particularly useful.  Illustrations similarly based on differences between the outcome rates of two groups at different ages include Table 1 of the Mortality and Survival page (concerning differences between black and white mortality/survival rates from oral and pharyngeal cancer at different ages), Table A to the Comment on Berrington de Gonzalez NEJM 2010 (concerning the effect of high body mass index on mortality/survival at different ages), and Table 6 of the 2008 Nordic Demographic Symposium presentation (concerning differences between renter and owner mortality/survival among Finnish men at different ages).

As explained in various places discussed or made available on the Measuring Health Disparities (MHD) and Scanlan’s Rule (SR) pages of jpscanlan.com, observed patterns of standard measures of differences between outcome rates (proportions) will be functions of both (a) the overall prevalence of an outcome and (b) the size of the difference between means of the underlying distributions of factors associated with experiencing the outcome.  The patterns by which the measures are affected by the prevalence of an outcome – which patterns are described most completely in the introductory section of SR – are functions of the shapes of the underlying distributions of factors associated with experiencing the outcome and I shall here refer to the forces driving those patterns as the distributional forces.

If the difference between the means of two groups’ distributions is the same in two settings, then one will generally – i.e., absent distributional regularities, including those addressed on the Truncation Issues sub-page of SR, or factors such as that described on the Irreducible Minimums sub-page of MHD – observe the patterns described in the introduction to SR.  Thus, for example, the setting with the lower prevalence of an outcome will generally show the larger relative difference in experiencing the outcome and the smaller relative difference in avoiding the outcome.  Also, when the outcome is uncommon in both settings (less than 50% for both groups) the absolute difference between rates will be higher in the setting with the greater prevalence;  when the outcome is common in both settings (more than 50% for both groups) the absolute difference between rates will be higher in the setting with the lower prevalence.  And patterns of comparative size of differences measured by odds ratios will be the opposite of those measured by absolute differences.

But where the difference between the group’s means is larger in one setting than another, for any given prevalence level, all measures of difference will be larger in the setting with the larger difference between means.  At different levels of prevalence the setting with the larger difference between means may or may not show larger differences for all measures.  That is, for example, if prevalence is lower in the setting with the larger difference between means, that setting will certainly exhibit the larger relative difference in experiencing the outcome, but may or may not exhibit the larger relative difference in avoiding the outcome.  This matter probably is discussed most fully in Race and Mortality (Society 2000), BSPS 2006, and Comment on Huijts EJPH 2009.  The matter is discussed with regard to reasons for differing expectations when the settings are differentiated temporally and differentiated otherwise in Section A.9 of SR. 

When discussing the role of the distributional forces, it is important to recognize that it would be a mistake to believe that the distributional forces are not present simply because they are not evident, as, for example, when both the relative difference between the rates at which two groups experience an outcome and the relative difference between the rates at which the two groups avoid the outcome are larger in one setting than the other.  Presumably, the distributional forces are operating but the size of the difference between means of the underlying distributions sufficiently large relative to the size of the difference in prevalence that the distributional forces are not evident. 

The main purpose of this item is to illustrate, using life table information, circumstances where the distributional forces will tend to predominate simply because the differences in prevalence are very large.  But I also illustrate that the issue of whether the difference in prevalence in the two settings is greater than the difference between means in the two setting can be approached scientifically.

As discussed in Section A.8 of SR, what I refer to as overall prevalence is not overall prevalence as the term would ordinarily be used in epidemiology, since strictly speaking overall prevalence is a function of the rates of each group and the proportion each group comprises of the total population.  I use the term to refer to a general pattern, as best identified with reference to the rate of the advantaged group at least where the advantaged group is the majority of the population.   One could as well use the disadvantaged group’s rate as the benchmark for overall prevalence. 

Using a particular groups’ rate as the benchmark for overall prevalence, one can measure differences in prevalence using the same approach that I have in various places maintained is the only useful measure of differences between outcome rates.  Such approach, which is described in the Solutions sub-page of MHD and for which a downloadable database is made available on the Solutions Database sub-page of MHD and which has existed in 1934 in the form of the probit, involves deriving from a pair of outcome rates the difference between underlying means measured in terms of the percentage of a standard deviation.  We can see from Table 1 of British Society for Population Studies 2006, for example, that where the advantaged and disadvantaged groups’ adverse outcome rates are 10.0% and 21.8%, the difference between means is .50 standard deviations.  Other illustrations may be found in the tables of British Society for Populations Studies 2008 and Nordic Demographic Symposium 2008.

Without getting into the details of the variation in differences between means, it is easy to understand that one situations where one will commonly observe the pattern whereby the rarer an outcome the greater the relative difference in experiencing it and the smaller the relative difference in avoiding involves examination of mortality rates by different age groups, at least when the age groups compared have substantially different mortality rates.

The point can be illustrated with the life table data on white and black men set out in Table LT1 below, which is an abbreviated, and simplified, portion of Table A the Life Table Information document.   The complete Table A also presents the same information for white men and women, white and black women, and black men and women.  As explained in the introduction to the Life Table Information document, the information in Table A is based on proportions of a cohort surviving /failing to survive to various ages.  As shown in Table B of the document, patterns based on proportions of persons surviving to one age who then survive/fail to survive to the next age, may differ in small ways.  And I note that the latter figures are more reflective of the proportions at issue when observers draw conclusions about patterns of effects of age or socioeconomic status on mortality.   But the information from Table A is suitable for instant purposes.

 

Table LT1 – Patterns of Measures of Differences between White and Black Rates of Failing to Survive to Various Ages [b1312 a 5][i]

Age

WM

BM

MR

SR

AbsDf

MOR

EES

20

1.30%

2.52%

1.94

1.01

1.22

1.96

0.28

30

2.63%

4.73%

1.80

1.02

2.10

1.84

0.27

40

4.15%

7.51%

1.81

1.04

3.36

1.88

0.30

50

7.35%

12.79%

1.74

1.06

5.45

1.85

0.32

60

13.96%

24.25%

1.74

1.14

10.30

1.97

0.39

70

27.03%

42.47%

1.57

1.27

15.44

1.99

0.44

80

51.93%

67.36%

1.30

1.47

15.43

1.91

0.41

90

84.68%

90.05%

1.06

1.54

5.37

1.64

0.26

100

99.20%

99.10%

1.00

0.89

-0.10

0.89

0.01

 

 

Compare the figures for white and black men dying by age 30 with the figures for those dying by age 60.  The difference between the prevalence of mortality/survival as to the two periods, based on the rate of the advantaged groups, is quite large.  Using the Solutions/Probit approach, one would find the 2.63% and the 13.96% mortality figures for white men to reflect an EES of .856 (i.e., difference between underlying means of .856 standard deviations).   Thus, even though the EES is larger for 60 than 30 (.39 versus .27), the mortality ratio is larger for age 30.

 

On the other hand, the difference in prevalence of mortality/survival is not as large for age 60 compared with 50 as age 60 compared with age 30.  The 7.35% and 13.96% figures for white men dying by ages 50 and 60 reflect an EES of .36.  Thus, the EES reflecting the difference in mortality between the advantaged and disadvantaged groups is sufficiently larger for age 60 than age 50 (.39 versus .32) to outweigh the tendency for the relative different in morality to be smaller at age 60 (where mortality is more common). 

 

Examples of these patterns involving data that researchers regarded as reflecting decreasing socioeconomic differences in mortality by age may be found in Tables 6 and 8 of British Society for Populations Studies 2008.  See also Tables1 and 2 of the Mortality and Survival page and Table A of Comment on Berrington de Gonzalez NEJM 2010, which involve interpretations of variation in other factors by age.

 



[i] The meaning of each columns in Table LT1 is as follows:

Age         Age

WM        Proportion of white men dying by age indicated

BM         Proportion of black men dying by age indicated

MR         Ratio of proportion of black men to proportion of white men dying by age indicated (BM/WM)

SR           Ratio of proportion of white men to proportion of black me surviving to age indicated – (1-WM)/(1-BM)

AbsDf    Percentage point difference in proportions of black men of dying by age indicated

MOR      Ratio of black male odds of dying by age indicated to white male odds of dying by age indicated                 

EES        Estimate effect size – difference between means of underlying distributions in terms of percentage of standard deviation