James P. Scanlan, Attorney at Law

Home Page

Curriculum Vitae

Publications

Published Articles

Conference Presentations

Working Papers

page1

Journal Comments

Truth in Justice Articles

Measurement Letters

Measuring Health Disp

Outline and Guide to MHD

Summary to MHD

Solutions

page3

Solutions Database

Irreducible Minimums

Pay for Performance

Between Group Variance

Concentration Index

Gini Coefficient

Reporting Heterogeneity

Cohort Considerations

Relative v Absolute Diff

Whitehall Studies

AHRQ's Vanderbilt Report

NHDR Measurement

NHDR Technical Issues

MHD A Articles

MHD B Conf Presentations

MHD D Journal Comments

Consensus/Non-Consensus

Spurious Contradictions

Institutional Corresp

page2

Scanlan's Rule

Outline and Guide to SR

Summary to SR

Bibliography

Semantic Issues

Employment Tests

Case Study

Case Study Answers

Case Study II

Subgroup Effects

Subgroup Effects NC

Illogical Premises

Illogical Premises II

Inevitable Interaction

Interactions by Age

Literacy Illustration

RERI

Feminization of Poverty S

Explanatory Theories

Mortality and Survival

Truncation Issues

Collected Illustrations

Income Illustrations

Framingham Illustrations

Life Table Illustrations

NHANES Illustrations

Mort/Surv Illustration

Credit Score Illustration

Intermediate Outcomes

Representational Disp

Statistical Signif SR

Comparing Averages

Meta-Analysis

Case Control Studies

Criminal Record Effects

Sears Case Illustration

Numeracy Illustration

Obesity Illusration

LIHTC Approval Disparitie

Recidivism Illustration

Consensus

Algorithm Fairness

Mortality and Survival 2

Mort/Survival Update

Measures of Association

Immunization Disparities

Race Health Initiative

Educational Disparities

Disparities by Subject

CUNY ISLG Eq Indicators

Harvard CRP NCLB Study

New York Proficiency Disp

Education Trust GC Study

Education Trust HA Study

AE Casey Profic Study

McKinsey Achiev Gap Study

California RICA

Nuclear Deterrence

Employment Discrimination

Job Segregation

Measuring Hiring Discr

Disparate Impact

Four-Fifths Rule

Less Discr Alt - Proc

Less Discr Altl - Subs

Fisher v. Transco Serv

Jones v. City of Boston

Bottom Line Issue

Lending Disparities

Inc & Cred Score Example

Disparities - High Income

Underadjustment Issues

Absolute Differences - L

Lathern v. NationsBank

US v. Countrywide

US v. Wells Fargo

Partial Picture Issues

Foreclosure Disparities

File Comparison Issues

FHA/VA Steering Study

CAP TARP Study

Disparities by Sector

Holder/Perez Letter

Federal Reserve Letter

Discipline Disparities

COPAA v. DeVos

Kerri K. V. California

Truancy Illustration

Disparate Treatment

Relative Absolute Diff

Offense Type Issues

Los Angeles SWPBS

Oakland Disparities

Richmond Disparities

Nashville Disparities

California Disparities

Denver Disparities

Colorado Disparities

Nor Carolina Disparitie

Aurora Disparities

Allegheny County Disp

Evansville Disparities

Maryland Disparities

St. Paul Disparities

Seattle Disparities

Minneapolis Disparities

Oregon Disparities

Beaverton Disparities

Montgomery County Disp

Henrico County Disparitie

Florida Disparities

Connecticut Disparities

Portland Disparities

Minnesota Disparities

Massachusetts Disparities

Rhode Island Disparities

South Bend Disparities

Utah Disparities

Loudoun Cty Disparities

Kern County Disparities

Milwaukee Disparities

Urbana Disparities

Illinois Disparities

Virginia Disparities

Behavior

Suburban Disparities

Preschool Disparities

Restraint Disparities

Disabilities - PL 108-446

Keep Kids in School Act

Gender Disparities

Ferguson Arrest Disp

NEPC Colorado Study

NEPC National Study

California Prison Pop

APA Zero Tolerance Study

Flawed Inferences - Disc

Oakland Agreement

DOE Equity Report

IDEA Data Center Guide

Duncan/Ali Letter

Crim Justice Disparities

U.S. Customs Search Disp

Deescalation Training

Career Criminal Study

Implicit Bias Training

Drawing Inferences

Diversion Programs

Minneapolis PD Investig

Offense Type Issues CJD

Innumerate Decree Monitor

Massachusetts CJ Disparit

Feminization of Poverty

Affirmative Action

Affirm Action for Women

Other Affirm Action

Justice John Paul Stevens

Statistical Reasoning

The Sears Case

Sears Case Documents

The AT&T Consent Decree

Cross v. ASPI

Vignettes

Times Higher Issues

Gender Diff in DADT Term

Adjustment Issues

Percentage Points

Odds Ratios

Statistical Signif Vig

Journalists & Statistics

Multiplication Definition

Prosecutorial Misconduct

Outline and Guide

Misconduct Summary

B1 Agent Cain Testimony

B1a Bev Wilsh Diversion

B2 Bk Entry re Cain Call

B3 John Mitchell Count

B3a Obscuring Msg Slips

B3b Missing Barksdale Int

B4 Park Towers

B5 Dean 1997 Motion

B6 Demery Testimony

B7 Sankin Receipts

B7a Sankin HBS App

B8 DOJ Complicity

B9 Doc Manager Complaints

B9a Fabricated Gov Exh 25

B11a DC Bar Complaint

Letters (Misconduct)

Links Page

Misconduct Profiles

Arlin M. Adams

Jo Ann Harris

Bruce C. Swartz

Swartz Addendum 2

Swartz Addendum 3

Swartz Addendum 4

Swartz Addendum 7

Robert E. O'Neill

O'Neill Addendum 7

Paula A. Sweeney

Robert J. Meyer

Lantos Hearings

Password Protected

OIC Doc Manager Material

DC Bar Materials

Temp Confidential

DV Issues

Indexes

Document Storage

Pre 1989

1989 - present

Presentations

Prosec Misc Docs

Prosec Misc Docs II

Profile PDFs

Misc Letters July 2008 on

Large Prosec Misc Docs

HUD Documents

Transcripts

Miscellaneous Documents

Unpublished Papers

Letters re MHD

Tables

MHD Comments

Figures

ASPI Documents

Web Page PDFs

Sears Documents

Pages Transfer


Education Trust Glass Ceiling Study

(April 26, 2014; rev. June 28, 2014)

This page is related to the Education Trust HA Study subpage of the Educational Disparities.  Like that subpage, this subpage involves an Education Trust study that attempted to evaluate educational disparities without an understanding of the extent to which observed patterns are functions of normal distributions.  The criticism of the Education Trust’s analyses on this subpage could as well be said of the health and healthcare disparities research of the Harvard School of Public Health, the Harvard Medical School, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention , the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, the National Center for Health Statistics, and the Institute of Medicine, as well as the virtually every other research institution in the world (as recently addressed at pages 24 to 32 of the Federal Committee on Statistical Methodology (FCSM) 2013 Research Conference paper “Measuring Health and Healthcare Disparities.”)    

Most subpages like this take for granted that the reader will have some familiarity with the statistical patterns described on scores of pages on this site.  In this case, however, I set out the basic patterns as they bear on the particular issues examined the Education Trust Glass Ceiling study – (a) falling below the basic level and (b) achieving the advanced level.  The rarer an outcome the greater tends to be the relative difference in experiencing it and the smaller tends to be the relative difference in avoiding it.  Thus, as is pertinent to the subject of this subpage, which involved a period where favorable outcomes were increasing the corresponding adverse outcomes were decreasing, (a) as an adverse educational outcome like falling below the basic proficiency level decreases in overall prevalence, relative differences in experiencing it tend to increase while relative differences in avoiding it tend to decrease; (b) as a favorable outcome like achieving the advanced level increases in overall prevalence relative differences in experiencing that outcome tend to decrease while relative in failing to experience it tend to increase.  It should be evident that (a) and (b) involve the same pattern viewed from different perspectives, since as one outcome increase the other necessarily decreases.  While prevalence related patterns of absolute differences are more difficult to describe, it suffices to note here that the rates of falling below basic are in ranges where decreases in the outcome will tend to reduce absolute differences between rates while the rates of achieving the advanced level are in ranges where increases in the rates will tend to increase absolute differences.  Fuller explanations of these patterns may be found in the introductory section to the Scanlan’s Rule, as well as, among many other places, in the FCSM paper mentioned above note and the October 9, 2012 Harvard University Measurement Letter.

Inasmuch as the Education Trust Glass Ceiling Study relied on absolute differences between rates, its approach may usefully be contrasted with the approach in the 2009 McKinsey & Company issued a report on the titled “The Economic Impact of the Achievement Gap in America’s Schools,” which discussed demographic differences in falling below the basic level in terms of relative difference in the adverse outcome rates (which will tend to increase as those outcomes are decreasing) and demographic differences in reaching the advanced level in terms of relative differences in achieving that level (which will tend to decrease as those outcomes increase).  Thus, the McKinsey approach would tend to reach opposite conclusions from the Education Trust approach. 

I also note here that there is a systematic tendency for the relative difference upon which an observer focuses to change in opposite directions from the absolute difference.  That occurs because the absolute difference tends to change in the same direction as the smaller relative difference.  But observers tend to focus on the larger relative difference simply because it provides the most striking disparity.

 

 

***

On May 14, 2014, the Education Trust issued a report styled “Breaking the Glass Ceiling of Achievement for Low-Income Students and Students of Color.”   The report (at 1) described itself as the first of a series attempting to shed light of racial/ethnic and income differences within the high end of the student populations.  The Education Trust HA Study discusses the April 1, 2014 study that is the second of the series.

The Glass Ceiling report attempted to appraise the size of racial/ethnic and income disparities in meeting or falling below certain proficiency levels based on absolute differences between rates.  The report, however, reflected no understanding of the patterns by which absolute differences between rates tend to change solely as a consequence of overall changes in rates.  Consequently, the report does not provide a sound analysis of whether racial/ethnic and income disparities have been increasing or decreasing.

The report’s most detailed analysis pertains to fourth graders.  One group of findings in the report related to racial/ethnic and income differences in failing to meet the basic level in math during a period from 2003 to 2011 when rates of reaching the basic level improved for all groups.  The report failed to recognize that the rates at issue were in ranges where general decreases in the rates examined would tend to reduce absolute differences regardless of whether there occurred a change in the comparative situation of the two groups. 

Table 1sets out the rates of failing to reach the basic level for the disadvantaged group (DG) and advantaged group (AG) in each comparison (indicated in the second column), along with ratio of AG’s rate of experiencing the favorable outcome (meeting basic) to DG’s rate of experiencing the favorable outcome (RRFav), the ratio of DG’s rate of experiencing the adverse outcome (falling below basic) to AG’s rate of experiencing the adverse outcome (RRAdv) and the absolute difference between rates (AD).  The table also shows the EES (for “estimated effect size”), which is a measure of the difference in circumstances of the two groups (in terms of number of standard deviations between mean scores of the hypothesized underlying distributions) that is unaffected by the prevalence of an outcome.  The table shows that each of the three standard measures behaved as it commonly does in the circumstance.   That is, the relative difference in the generally increasing (favorable) outcome decreased; the relative difference in the generally decreasing (adverse) outcome increased; and the absolute difference decreased.  The EES indicates that, to the extent that the difference in the circumstances of the two groups reflected by each pair rates can be measured, it declined.

Table 1.  Rates at which disadvantaged groups (DG) and advantaged groups (DG) in fourth grade fell belowbasic level in math in 2003 and 2011, with measures of differences.

 

Issue

Compariosn

Yr

DG

AG

RRFav

RRAdv

AD

EES

Below basic

Low v high income

2003

38.00%

12.00%

1.42

3.17

0.26

0.87

Below basic

Low v high income

2011

27.00%

8.00%

1.26

3.38

0.19

0.79

Below basic

Black v white

2003

46.00%

13.00%

1.61

3.54

0.33

1.02

Below basic

Black v white

2011

34.00%

9.00%

1.38

3.78

0.25

0.92

Below basic

Hispanic v white

2003

38.00%

13.00%

1.40

2.92

0.25

0.82

Below basic

Hispanic v white

2011

28.00%

9.00%

1.26

3.11

0.19

0.75

 

The report’s conclusion as to decreasing disparities was thus broadly correct.  But its implication that the absolute difference was a sound measure of disparity in the context examined is incorrect. 

A notable reflection of the failure to understand the patterns be which relative as well as absolute differences tend to be affected by the prevalence of an outcome may be found in the report’s reference to a relative difference as a measure of disparity.  After stating that the decline in absolute differences between rates reflected significant progress, the report states (at 6):   “Still, black, Hispanic, and low-income students were more than three times as likely as their peers to perform within the lowest achievement category in 2011.”  But, as shown in RRAdv column, rather than failing to decline more (as the characterization in the report implies), the relative difference in the adverse outcome increased.

The report also relied on absolute differences to conclude that while most groups increased their rates of reaching the advanced level, disparities increased.  Here, too, the report failed to recognize that the rates were in ranges (well below 50%) where general increases would tend to increase absolute differences.  In this case, however, there was less consistent a pattern of increases for all groups (than in the case of decreases in rates of falling below basic).  Thus, a pattern of increasing disparities could be found for all standard indicators (in which case the EES also would necessarily increase). 

But one exception regarding the Hispanic-white disparity warrants note.  The first two rows of Table 2 present figures discussed in the report for comparison of Hispanics with whites.  In this case, all standard measures changed as they typically do in the circumstances.  And the EES shows an increase in disparity.  But it was a very small increase (from .69 to .71 standard deviations), rather than the fairly large increase suggested by the increase in the absolute difference from 4 to 7 percentage points.

Table 2.  Rates at which Hispanics and whites in fourth grade reached advanced level in math in 2003 and 2011, with measures of differences. [ref b5225a4]

 

Issue

Numbers

Yr

His

White

RRFav

RRAdv

AD

EES

Advanced

Rounded

2003

1.00%

5.00%

5.00

1.04

0.04

0.69

Advanced

Rounded

2011

2.00%

9.00%

4.50

1.08

0.07

0.71

Advanced

Refined

2003

0.80%

5.20%

6.50

1.05

0.044

0.78

Advanced

Refined

2011

2.00%

8.90%

4.45

1.08

0.067

0.70

 

In this case, however, the rates were low enough that the rounding to the nearest percent employed in the report could make a difference.[i]  Thus, the second and third rows of Table 2 show the Hispanic and white rates of reaching the advanced level carried out to a tenth of a percent (as estimated based on the lines in Figure 1b at page 5).  Using these rates, we observe that while again all standard measures behaved as they commonly do in the circumstance, the EES indicates that the disparity decreased (from .78 to .70 standard deviations). 

The report also relies on absolute differences to make a point that the widening of racial and ethnic gaps in reaching advanced level math was more pronounced among higher income groups than lower income groups.  But as shown by the EES figures in Table 3, with respect to the Hispanic-white comparisons (the only comparison for which there are data in Figure 2b at 7), while the gap widened for the lower income group it, decreased for the higher income group.  Thus, not only was there not a more pronounced increase in the gap within the higher income group than the lower income group, to the extent that the gap can be effectively measured, it declined in the higher income group. 

Table 3.  Rates at which Hispanics and white in fourth grade reached advanced level in math in 2003 and 2011, by income level, with measures of differences.

 

Issue

Income Level

Yr

Hisp

White

RRFav

RRAdv

AD

EES

Advanced

Low

2003

0.80%

1.50%

1.88

1.01

0.01

0.23

Advanced

Low

2011

1.50%

3.20%

2.13

1.02

0.02

0.31

Advanced

High

2003

2.00%

6.00%

3.00

1.04

0.04

0.50

Advanced

High

2011

5.00%

12.00%

2.40

1.08

0.07

0.47

 

The report also discusses some gaps among 12th graders for the years 2005 and 2009, again relying on absolute differences.  That discussion also suffers from the failure to recognize the way absolute differences tend to change simply because the overall prevalence of an outcome changes. 



[i]  A number of the report’s endnotes discuss the importance of rounding issues in the context of some of the very low rates at issue.